Mon compte

connexion

inscription

   Publicité R▼


 » 
allemand anglais arabe bulgare chinois coréen croate danois espagnol espéranto estonien finnois français grec hébreu hindi hongrois islandais indonésien italien japonais letton lituanien malgache néerlandais norvégien persan polonais portugais roumain russe serbe slovaque slovène suédois tchèque thai turc vietnamien
allemand anglais arabe bulgare chinois coréen croate danois espagnol espéranto estonien finnois français grec hébreu hindi hongrois islandais indonésien italien japonais letton lituanien malgache néerlandais norvégien persan polonais portugais roumain russe serbe slovaque slovène suédois tchèque thai turc vietnamien

Significations et usages de Pareidolia

Définition

pareidolia (n.)

1.the tendency for incorrect perception of a stimulus as an object, pattern or meaning known to the observer, such as seeing shapes in clouds, seeing faces in inanimate objects or abstract patterns, or hearing hidden messages in music.

   Publicité ▼

Définition (complément)

⇨ voir la définition de Wikipedia

Dictionnaire analogique

pareidolia (n.)

apophenia[Hyper.]


   Publicité ▼

Wikipedia

Pareidolia

                   
  A satellite photo of a mesa in Cydonia, often called the Face on Mars. Later imagery from other angles did not contain the illusion.

Pareidolia (play /pærɨˈdliə/ parr-i-DOH-lee-ə) is a psychological phenomenon involving a vague and random stimulus (often an image or sound) being perceived as significant. Common examples include seeing images of animals or faces in clouds, the man in the moon or the Moon rabbit, and hearing hidden messages on records played in reverse.

The word comes from the Greek words para (παρά, "beside, alongside, instead") in this context meaning something faulty, wrong, instead of; and the noun eidōlon (εἴδωλον "image, form, shape") the diminutive of eidos. Pareidolia is a type of apophenia.

Contents

  Examples

  Religious

There have been many instances of perceptions of religious imagery and themes, especially the faces of religious figures, in ordinary phenomena. Many involve images of Jesus, the Virgin Mary, or the word Allah.[1]

In 1978, a New Mexican woman found that the burn marks on a tortilla she had made appeared similar to the traditional western depiction of Jesus Christ's face. Thousands of people came to see the framed tortilla.[2]

Publicity surrounding sightings of religious figures and other surprising images in ordinary objects has spawned a market for such items on online auctions like eBay. One famous instance was a grilled cheese sandwich with the "Virgin Mary"'s face.[3]

In September, 2007, the so-called "monkey tree phenomenon" caused a minor social mania in Singapore. A callus on a tree resembled a monkey, and believers flocked to the tree to pay homage to the "Monkey god" (either Sun Wukong or Hanuman).[4]

  Divination

Various European ancient divination practices involve the interpretation of shadows cast by objects. For example, in Nordic molybdomancy, a random shape produced by pouring molten tin into cold water is interpreted by the shadow it casts in candlelight.

  Fossils

From the late 1970s through the early 1980s, Japanese researcher Chonosuke Okamura self-published a famous series of reports titled "Original Report of the Okamura Fossil Laboratory" in which he described tiny inclusions in polished limestone from the Silurian period (425 mya) as being preserved fossil remains of tiny humans, gorillas, dogs, dragons, dinosaurs, and other organisms, all of them only millimeters long, leading him to claim "There have been no changes in the bodies of mankind since the Silurian period ... except for a growth in stature from 3.5 mm to 1,700 mm."[5][6] Okamura's research earned him a winner of the Ig Nobel Prize (a parody of the Nobel Prizes) in biodiversity.[7] See List of Ig Nobel Prize winners (1996).[8]

  Projective tests

The Rorschach inkblot test uses pareidolia in an attempt to gain insight into a person's mental state. The Rorschach is a projective test, as it intentionally elicits the thoughts or feelings of respondents which are "projected" onto the ambiguous inkblot images. Projection in this instance is a form of "directed pareidolia" because the cards have been deliberately designed not to resemble anything in particular.[2]

  Electronic voice phenomenon

In 1971, Konstantin Raudive wrote Breakthrough, detailing what he believed was the discovery of electronic voice phenomenon (EVP). EVP has been described as auditory pareidolia.[2]

  Backmasking

The allegations of backmasking in popular music have also been described as pareidolia.[2][9]

  Explanations

  Evolutionary advantage

  A drawing which, despite not bearing much resemblance to a real face, most people will identify as a picture of one.

Carl Sagan hypothesized that as a survival technique, human beings are "hard-wired" from birth to identify the human face. This allows people to use only minimal details to recognize faces from a distance and in poor visibility but can also lead them to interpret random images or patterns of light and shade as being faces.[10] The evolutionary advantages of being able to identify friend from foe with split-second accuracy are numerous; prehistoric (and even modern) men and women who accidentally identify an enemy as a friend could face deadly consequences for this mistake. This is only one among many evolutionary pressures responsible for the development of the facial recognition capability of modern humans.[11]

In Cosmos: A Personal Voyage Sagan claimed that Heikegani crabs' occasional resemblance to Samurai resulted in their being spared from capture and thus exaggerate the trait in their offspring, a hypothesis proposed by Julian Huxley in 1952. Such claims have been met with skepticism.[12]

A 2009 magnetoencephalography study found that objects incidentally perceived as faces evoke an early (165 ms) activation in the ventral fusiform cortex, at a time and location similar to that evoked by faces, whereas other common objects do not evoke such activation. This activation is similar to a slightly earlier peak at 130 ms seen for images of real faces. The authors suggest that face perception evoked by face-like objects is a relatively early process, and not a late cognitive reinterpretation phenomenon.[13] An fMRI study in 2011 similarly showed that repeated presentation of novel visual shapes that were interpreted as meaningful led to decreased fMRI responses for real objects. These result indicate that interpretation of ambiguous stimuli depends on similar processes as those elicited for known objects.[14]

These studies help to explain why people identify a few circles and a line as a "face" so quickly and without hesitation. Cognitive processes are activated by the "face-like" object, which alert the observer to both the emotional state and identity of the subject - even before the conscious mind begins to process - or even receive - the information. The "stick figure face," despite its simplicity, conveys mood information (in this case, disappointment or mild unhappiness). It would be just as simple to draw a stick figure face that would be perceived (by most people) as hostile and aggressive. This robust and subtle capability is the result of eons of natural selection favoring people most able to quickly identify the mental state, for example, of threatening people, thus providing the individual an opportunity to flee and fight another day. In other words, processing this information subcortically (and therefore subconsciously) - before it is passed on to the rest of the brain for detailed processing - accelerates judgment and decision making when alacrity is paramount.[11] This ability, though highly specialized for the processing and recognition of human emotions, also functions to determine the demeanor of wildlife.[15]

Combined with Apophenia (seeing patterns in randomness) and hierophany (a manifestation of the sacred), pareidolia may have helped early societies organize chaos and make the world intelligible.[16][17]

  Pathologies

There are a number of conditions that can cause an individual to lose his/her ability to recognize faces; stroke, tumors, and trauma to the ventral fusiform gyrus are the most common culprits. This is known as prosopagnosia. Pareidolia can also be related to obsessive–compulsive disorder as seen in one woman's case[18]

  Natural

  Artificial

Pareidolia examples
A box
This alarm clock appears to have a sad face.  
refer to caption
False wood with multiple pareidolia aspects.  
A rusty piece of machinery looks like the face of a beast.  
A cardboard box that appears to be shocked and unhappy  

  See also

  References

  1. ^ Ibrahim, Yahaya (2011-01-02). "In Maiduguri, a tree with engraved name of God turns spot to a Mecca of sorts". Sunday Trust (Media Trust Limited, Abuja). http://sundaytrust.com.ng/index.php?option=com_content&view=article&id=5698:in-maiduguri-a-tree-with-engraved-name-of-god-turns-spot-to-a-mecca-of-sorts&catid=17:community-news-kanem-trust&Itemid=28. Retrieved 2012-03-21. 
  2. ^ a b c d Zusne, Leonard; Warren H. Jones (1989). Anomalistic Psychology: A Study of Magical Thinking. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 77–79. ISBN 0-8058-0508-7. http://books.google.com/books?isbn=0805805087. Retrieved 2007-04-06. 
  3. ^ "'Virgin Mary' toast fetches $28,000". BBC News. 23 November 2004. http://news.bbc.co.uk/2/hi/americas/4034787.stm. Retrieved 2006-10-27. 
  4. ^ Ng Hui Hui (13 September 2007). "Monkey See, Monkey Do?". The New Paper. pp. 12–13. http://newpaper.asia1.com.sg/printfriendly/0,4139,141806,00.html. [dead link]
  5. ^ Spamer, E.. "Chonosuke Okamura, Visionary". Philadelphia: Academy of Natural Sciences. http://improbable.com/airchives/paperair/volume6/v6i6/okamura-6-6.html.  archived at Improbable Research
  6. ^ Berenbaum, May (2009). The earwig's tail: a modern bestiary of multi-legged legends. Harvard University Press. pp. 72–73. ISBN 0-674-03540-2. 
  7. ^ Marc Abrahams (2004-03-16). "Tiny tall tales: Marc Abrahams uncovers the minute, but astonishing, evidence of our fossilised past". London: The Guardian. http://www.guardian.co.uk/education/2004/mar/16/highereducation.research. 
  8. ^ Conner, Susan; Kitchen, Linda (2002). Science's most wanted: the top 10 book of outrageous innovators, deadly disasters, and shocking discoveries. Most Wanted Series. Brassey's. p. 93. ISBN 1-57488-481-6. 
  9. ^ Vokey, John R; J. Don Read (November 1985). "Sublminal message: between the devil and the media". American Psychologist. 11 40 (11): 1231–1239. DOI:10.1037/0003-066X.40.11.1231. PMID 4083611. 
  10. ^ Sagan, Carl (1995). The Demon-Haunted World - Science as a Candle in the Dark. New York: Random House. ISBN 0-394-53512-X. 
  11. ^ a b Svoboda, Elizabeth (2007-02-13). "Facial Recognition - Brain - Faces, Faces Everywhere". The New York Times (New York Times). http://www.nytimes.com/2007/02/13/health/psychology/13face.html. Retrieved July 3, 2010. 
  12. ^ Joel W. Martin (1993). "The Samurai Crab" (PDF). Terra 31 (4): 30–34. http://crustacea.nhm.org/people/martin/publications/pdf/103.pdf. 
  13. ^ Hadjikhani N, Kveraga K, Naik P, Ahlfors SP (February 2009). "Early (M170) activation of face-specific cortex by face-like objects". Neuroreport 20 (4): 403–7. DOI:10.1097/WNR.0b013e328325a8e1. PMC 2713437. PMID 19218867. //www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?tool=pmcentrez&artid=2713437. 
  14. ^ Voss JL, Federmeier KD, Paller, K (2011). "The potato chip really does look like Elvis! Neural hallmarks of conceptual processing associated with finding novel shapes subjectively meaningful". Cerebral Cortex. DOI:10.1093/cercor/bhr315. PMID 22079921. http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/content/early/2011/11/10/cercor.bhr315. 
  15. ^ "Dog Tips - Emotions in Canines and Humans". Partnership for Animal Welfare. http://www.paw-rescue.org/PAW/PETTIPS/DogTip_EmotionsInCaninesAndHumans.php. Retrieved July 3, 2010. 
  16. ^ [Bustamante Patricio, Yao Fay, Bustamante Daniela, 2010 b, The Worship to the Mountains: A Study of the Creation Myths of the Chinese Culture, http://www.rupestreweb.info/china.html]
  17. ^ [Bustamante Patricio, Yao Fay & Bustamante Daniela, 2010 c, From Pleistocene Art to the Worship of the Mountains in China. Methodological tools for Mimesis in Paleoart, Congress IFRAO 2010 - 'Pleistocene Art of the World'. Symposium. Signs, Symbols, Myth, Ideology. Pleistocene Art: the archeological material and its anthropological meanings. http://www.ifraoariege2010.fr/docs/Articles/Bustamante_et_al-Signes.pdf]
  18. ^ Fontenelle, Leonardo. "Leonardo F. Fontenelle. Pareidolias in obsessive-compulsive disorder". http://www.ingentaconnect.com/content/psych/nncs/2008/00000014/00000005/art00004. Retrieved October 28, 2011. 

  External links

   
               

 

Toutes les traductions de Pareidolia


Contenu de sensagent

  • définitions
  • synonymes
  • antonymes
  • encyclopédie

dictionnaire et traducteur pour sites web

Alexandria

Une fenêtre (pop-into) d'information (contenu principal de Sensagent) est invoquée un double-clic sur n'importe quel mot de votre page web. LA fenêtre fournit des explications et des traductions contextuelles, c'est-à-dire sans obliger votre visiteur à quitter votre page web !

Essayer ici, télécharger le code;

SensagentBox

Avec la boîte de recherches Sensagent, les visiteurs de votre site peuvent également accéder à une information de référence pertinente parmi plus de 5 millions de pages web indexées sur Sensagent.com. Vous pouvez Choisir la taille qui convient le mieux à votre site et adapter la charte graphique.

Solution commerce électronique

Augmenter le contenu de votre site

Ajouter de nouveaux contenus Add à votre site depuis Sensagent par XML.

Parcourir les produits et les annonces

Obtenir des informations en XML pour filtrer le meilleur contenu.

Indexer des images et définir des méta-données

Fixer la signification de chaque méta-donnée (multilingue).


Renseignements suite à un email de description de votre projet.

Jeux de lettres

Les jeux de lettre français sont :
○   Anagrammes
○   jokers, mots-croisés
○   Lettris
○   Boggle.

Lettris

Lettris est un jeu de lettres gravitationnelles proche de Tetris. Chaque lettre qui apparaît descend ; il faut placer les lettres de telle manière que des mots se forment (gauche, droit, haut et bas) et que de la place soit libérée.

boggle

Il s'agit en 3 minutes de trouver le plus grand nombre de mots possibles de trois lettres et plus dans une grille de 16 lettres. Il est aussi possible de jouer avec la grille de 25 cases. Les lettres doivent être adjacentes et les mots les plus longs sont les meilleurs. Participer au concours et enregistrer votre nom dans la liste de meilleurs joueurs ! Jouer

Dictionnaire de la langue française
Principales Références

La plupart des définitions du français sont proposées par SenseGates et comportent un approfondissement avec Littré et plusieurs auteurs techniques spécialisés.
Le dictionnaire des synonymes est surtout dérivé du dictionnaire intégral (TID).
L'encyclopédie française bénéficie de la licence Wikipedia (GNU).

Copyright

Les jeux de lettres anagramme, mot-croisé, joker, Lettris et Boggle sont proposés par Memodata.
Le service web Alexandria est motorisé par Memodata pour faciliter les recherches sur Ebay.
La SensagentBox est offerte par sensAgent.

Traduction

Changer la langue cible pour obtenir des traductions.
Astuce: parcourir les champs sémantiques du dictionnaire analogique en plusieurs langues pour mieux apprendre avec sensagent.

 

11483 visiteurs en ligne

calculé en 0,031s


Je voudrais signaler :
section :
une faute d'orthographe ou de grammaire
un contenu abusif (raciste, pornographique, diffamatoire)
une violation de copyright
une erreur
un manque
autre
merci de préciser :