Mon compte

connexion

inscription

   Publicité R▼


 » 
allemand anglais arabe bulgare chinois coréen croate danois espagnol espéranto estonien finnois français grec hébreu hindi hongrois islandais indonésien italien japonais letton lituanien malgache néerlandais norvégien persan polonais portugais roumain russe serbe slovaque slovène suédois tchèque thai turc vietnamien
allemand anglais arabe bulgare chinois coréen croate danois espagnol espéranto estonien finnois français grec hébreu hindi hongrois islandais indonésien italien japonais letton lituanien malgache néerlandais norvégien persan polonais portugais roumain russe serbe slovaque slovène suédois tchèque thai turc vietnamien

Significations et usages de Reconstruction_of_automobile_destruction

Définition

⇨ voir la définition de Wikipedia

   Publicité ▼

Wikipedia

Reconstruction of automobile destruction

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Jump to: navigation, search

Reconstruction of automobile destruction was a psychological study carried out by Elizabeth Loftus and Palmer in 1974.[1] The aim of the study was to investigate whether or not an eye witness's memory can be altered by information supplied to them after an event. Loftus and Palmer also wished to discover whether or not a person's memory can be influenced by this information.

Contents

Background to the study

The purpose of the study was to determine how memory is influenced by circumstances and prompting surrounding memory storage and recall. Previous studies had established that memories were not necessarily accurate representations of actual events, but were actually constructed using past experiences and other influences.

Participants

Two studies were carried out by Loftus & Palmer. The first was tested on forty-five students split into five categories, each with nine students. The second was carried out on one hundred and fifty students.

Method of study one

In the first study forty-five students from the University of Washington were shown seven film clips of car accidents. The clips ranged from five to thirty seconds long. After viewing each clip the students were asked to write a report on what they had seen. They were asked a series of questions about the videos. The critical question in this study was "At what speed was the car traveling?" The five categories of students were asked this question but with a different verb (shown below). Loftus and Palmer wanted to see if the verb influenced the students (eye witnesses') answers.

  1. About how fast were the cars going when they collided with each other?
  2. About how fast were the cars going when they smashed into each other?
  3. About how fast were the cars going when they bumped into each other?
  4. About how fast were the cars going when they hit each other?
  5. About how fast were the cars going when they contacted each other?

Results of study one

Loftus and Palmer found that the more intense the verb used in the question, the higher the estimate of speed. The actual results were:

Smashed : 40.8 miles per hour

Collided : 39.3 miles per hour

Bumped: 38.1 miles per hour

Hit : 34 miles per hour

Contacted : 31.8 miles per hour

Loftus and Palmer explained that the more severe the verb the more likely we are to estimate a higher speed. However they also argued that the findings could simply be due to a distorted memory caused by the verb that was used. They stated that some answers may have been due to a response bias meaning people who could not remember simply devised a made up answer.

Method of study two

In the second study, one hundred and fifty students watched a one minute video clip which contained a four second scene of a multiple car pile-up. They were then asked a series of questions about the scene. The same question ("about how fast were the cars going...") was used but another critical question was asked: "Was there any broken glass at the scene?". In this study there were three groups of students. Each group was asked a different version of the question concerning the speed of the automobiles. One group was asked "About how fast were the cars going when they smashed into each other?", the second was asked "About what speed were the cars going when they hit each other?" and finally the last group were not asked the question about speed. (km/h)

Results of study two

In study two Loftus and Palmer discovered that the intensity of the verb affected the students opinion on whether there was any glass on the scene. Actually there was no glass present but by challenging the students memory Loftus and Palmer influenced the students to believe otherwise. The actual results were:

Smashed : 16 students claimed they could remember seeing broken glass whereas 34 said there was none present.

Hit : 7 students remembered glass but 43 were correct in thinking there was none.

No Question: 6 students could recall broken glass and 44 could not.

From their study Loftus and Palmer concluded that the verb also played a part in the misperception of broken glass in the film.

See also

References

 

Toutes les traductions de Reconstruction_of_automobile_destruction


Contenu de sensagent

  • définitions
  • synonymes
  • antonymes
  • encyclopédie

dictionnaire et traducteur pour sites web

Alexandria

Une fenêtre (pop-into) d'information (contenu principal de Sensagent) est invoquée un double-clic sur n'importe quel mot de votre page web. LA fenêtre fournit des explications et des traductions contextuelles, c'est-à-dire sans obliger votre visiteur à quitter votre page web !

Essayer ici, télécharger le code;

SensagentBox

Avec la boîte de recherches Sensagent, les visiteurs de votre site peuvent également accéder à une information de référence pertinente parmi plus de 5 millions de pages web indexées sur Sensagent.com. Vous pouvez Choisir la taille qui convient le mieux à votre site et adapter la charte graphique.

Solution commerce électronique

Augmenter le contenu de votre site

Ajouter de nouveaux contenus Add à votre site depuis Sensagent par XML.

Parcourir les produits et les annonces

Obtenir des informations en XML pour filtrer le meilleur contenu.

Indexer des images et définir des méta-données

Fixer la signification de chaque méta-donnée (multilingue).


Renseignements suite à un email de description de votre projet.

Jeux de lettres

Les jeux de lettre français sont :
○   Anagrammes
○   jokers, mots-croisés
○   Lettris
○   Boggle.

Lettris

Lettris est un jeu de lettres gravitationnelles proche de Tetris. Chaque lettre qui apparaît descend ; il faut placer les lettres de telle manière que des mots se forment (gauche, droit, haut et bas) et que de la place soit libérée.

boggle

Il s'agit en 3 minutes de trouver le plus grand nombre de mots possibles de trois lettres et plus dans une grille de 16 lettres. Il est aussi possible de jouer avec la grille de 25 cases. Les lettres doivent être adjacentes et les mots les plus longs sont les meilleurs. Participer au concours et enregistrer votre nom dans la liste de meilleurs joueurs ! Jouer

Dictionnaire de la langue française
Principales Références

La plupart des définitions du français sont proposées par SenseGates et comportent un approfondissement avec Littré et plusieurs auteurs techniques spécialisés.
Le dictionnaire des synonymes est surtout dérivé du dictionnaire intégral (TID).
L'encyclopédie française bénéficie de la licence Wikipedia (GNU).

Copyright

Les jeux de lettres anagramme, mot-croisé, joker, Lettris et Boggle sont proposés par Memodata.
Le service web Alexandria est motorisé par Memodata pour faciliter les recherches sur Ebay.
La SensagentBox est offerte par sensAgent.

Traduction

Changer la langue cible pour obtenir des traductions.
Astuce: parcourir les champs sémantiques du dictionnaire analogique en plusieurs langues pour mieux apprendre avec sensagent.

 

12260 visiteurs en ligne

calculé en 0,032s


Je voudrais signaler :
section :
une faute d'orthographe ou de grammaire
un contenu abusif (raciste, pornographique, diffamatoire)
une violation de copyright
une erreur
un manque
autre
merci de préciser :